Skip to main content

Preceding psychological factors and calorie intake in patients with type 2 diabetes: investigation by ecological momentary assessment

Abstract

Background

Psychological factors have been reported to have influence on the eating habits of patients with diabetes. However, previous studies have used questionnaires to investigate the association, and thus include recall bias. To overcome this disadvantage, ecological momentary assessment (EMA) can be used to record subjective symptoms and behavior in subjects’ daily lives. Therefore, the aim of the present study was to investigate the influence of preceding psychological factors on calorie intake using computerized EMA for 6 months.

Methods

The participants were nine outpatients with type 2 diabetes, aged 34–72. They were instructed to use a personal digital assistant as an electronic diary for 6 months to record subjective symptoms, such as psychological stress, anxiety, and depressive mood, and the food and drink that they consumed. The association between a preceding psychological factor and calorie intake within 5 hours was investigated using multilevel modeling.

Results

Preceding psychological stress was positively associated with calorie intake from snacks. Preceding psychological stress, anxiety, and depressive mood were negatively associated with calorie intake from regular meals.

Conclusions

Preceding psychological factors influence the calorie intake of patients with type 2 diabetes. Understanding the role of these factors will be useful for developing psychological interventions to prevent overeating.

Trial registration

The trial registration number: UMIN000002992. Date of registration: 2010/01/07.

Background

Psychological factors, such as anxiety and depressive mood, have been reported to have influence on the eating habits of patients with life-style diseases, such as diabetes, which raises the risks of cardiovascular diseases [1]. Many studies have used questionnaires to investigate the association between psychological factors and eating habits, as in this previous study. However, the use of questionnaires is problematic because of their potential for recall bias [2]. To overcome this disadvantage, ecological momentary aqssessment (EMA), which avoids recall bias, has been used in the area of behavioral medicine to record subjective symptoms and behavior in subjects’ daily lives.

Some previous studies have been done to investigate the association between psychosocial factors and food intake using EMA [3,4,5,6]; however, these studies included methodological problems. First, most [3,4,5] used paper-and-pencil diaries to record subjective symptoms and food intake, while Tomiyama et al. [6] used an electronic diary (i.e. computerized EMA). Compared with EMA using paper-and-pencil diaries, computerized EMA has the advantage of avoiding ‘faked compliance’, i.e. disguised compliance by recording data at times other than those designated, by recording the time of data input [7]. In addition, the study by Tomiyama, which used computerized EMA, only assessed two days.

The aim of the present study was to do a long-term, quantitative investigation of the influence of preceding psychological factors on calorie intake, using computerized EMA for six months.

Methods

Participants and procedures

The participants were nine outpatients with type 2 diabetes diagnosed according to the current criteria of the Japan Diabetes Society [8] (three men; median age = 49 years, range = 34 to 72) who also took part in another study [9]. Inclusion criteria were age ≥ 20 and an outpatient with type 2 diabetes at the University of Tokyo Hospital. Participants were excluded if they had HbA1c ≥ 8.4% (corresponding to 8.0% before converting to the NGSP value, which was the criterion for poorly controlled type 2 diabetes), had any active mental disorder, followed a restrictive diet with protein ≤0.5 g/kg/day or salt ≤5 g/day due to another disease, had cognitive impairment, or had severe complications that interfered with self-care activities. After providing written informed consent, participants were instructed to record daily dietary intake using a PDA-based food diary, SELFOOD [10], and to record mood states, such as anxiety and depression, on a personal digital assistant (PDA) (WS020SH, Sharp Corp, Osaka, Japan, 135 g, 50x116x17.9 mm, Windows Mobile 6.1) that was equipped with a 3.0-in color liquid crystal display and a touch panel manipulated by a finger. They were asked to record as soon as possible after eating for 6 consecutive months. Anthropometric measurements, including height, body weight, and blood pressure, were performed at the beginning of the study. Clinical data (e.g. fasting plasma glucose level, hemoglobin A1c value, and duration of diabetes) were collected at their most recent visit with a health care provider. They visited their physician regularly, and continued to take the medications prescribed for their conditions during the study period.

Dietary intake

We developed an electronic food diary, “SELFOOD”, for use in a personal digital assistant (PDA). Its accuracy has been confirmed by comparison with a 24-h recall interview by registered dietitians [10]. SELFOOD includes a reference database with 423 color photographs of food and drink items and allows patients to add their own meals and recipes to the database if their nutrient information is given. SELFOOD calculates energy intake after every input. Calorie intake was summed on an input-by-input basis and converted to a daily bar graph with a horizontal line indicating the target value of daily calorie intake.

Momentary psychological stress and mood states

A PDA was used as electronic diary to record psychological stress, anxiety, and depressive mood [11]. The subjects were instructed on the use of the device and given manuals before the beginning of the study period. In addition, they practiced manipulating the device with the author until they became accustomed to its use.

The subjects used the PDAs for six consecutive months. Signal-contingent recordings were defined as recordings that were prompted with a beep as a signal [2], which was programmed to occur randomly within an interval of 30 mins, around 10:00 and 15:00. If the subjects did not enter a recording when the computer beeped, they were allowed to postpone input for 30 min. Recordings not made within 30 min were cancelled. The subjects were also asked to record their psychological stress, anxiety, and depressive mood when they woke up and when they went to bed by choosing “waking up” or “going to bed” from the menu. After selecting a “going to bed” recording, to avoid sleep disturbance the computers suspended the signal-contingent recordings until a selected “wake up” recording was played. Signal-contingent recordings and recordings when waking up and going to bed were treated as scheduled recordings. Momentary psychological stress, anxiety, and depressive mood were rated with a visual analog scale (VAS) that ranged from 0 to 100, which was displayed on the screen.

Statistical analysis

Multilevel modeling was used for statistical analyses to investigate preceding psychological factors on dietary intake because the dataset in this study had a nested structure in which a number of recordings belonged to each participant. Pairs of preceding psychological factors and dietary intake were lagged in the range of − 5 to 0 h [12]. The level of significance was set at 0.05. SAS Proc Mixed (SAS 9.3, SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA) was used. Each dietary intake recording was paired with the preceding momentary recording of psychological stress and mood states. Dietary intake was treated as the dependent variable. Momentary psychological stress, anxiety, and depressive mood were separately modeled as the independent variable, either as a fixed or random effect. Preceding momentary recordings of psychological factors were recorded within five hours before dietary intake, and there was no dietary intake between a preceding momentary recording of psychological factors and the dependent variable of dietary intake. Using these stratified time lags, models with different combinations of independent variables (with/without a preceding psychological factor as a fixed/random effect) were tested. Goodness of fit was compared to determine the best-fit model with a − 2 log likelihood function and χ2 test when one model was nested in the other; otherwise, Akaike’s Information Criterion (AIC) was used. The level-1 (within-individual level) intercept was modeled as a random effect. In all analyses, the variance-covariance matrix (G matrix) was modeled as unstructured.

Results

Subject characteristics

The characteristics of subjects are shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Subject characteristics

Recording profiles

In total, the data from 1353 days of recordings was available for analysis. The compliance rate for regular dietary intake was 77.2%, while the compliance rate for signal-contingent recordings of psychological factors was 42.8%. With regard to pairing dietary intake and preceding psychological factors, 1517 of 3667 dietary intakes could be paired with preceding psychological factors.

Preceding psychological stress and dietary calorie intake

Preceding psychological stress was significantly, negatively associated with dietary intake for lunch and dinner, but positively associated for snacks (Tables 2, 3 and 4). The final model is as follows:

Table 2 Effect of preceding psychological stress on calorie intake, for lunch
Table 3 Effect of preceding psychological stress on calorie intake, for dinner
Table 4 Effect of preceding psychological stress on calorie intake, for snacks

Level 1 equation:

$$ {\mathrm{Y}}_{\mathrm{ij}}={\uppi}_{0\mathrm{i}}+{\uppi}_{1\mathrm{i}}{\mathrm{Company}}_{\mathrm{ij}}+{\uppi}_{2\mathrm{i}}{\mathrm{Place}}_{\mathrm{ij}}+{\uppi}_{3\mathrm{i}}{\mathrm{Stress}}_{\mathrm{ij}}+{\upvarepsilon}_{\mathrm{ij}} $$

Level 2 equation:

$$ {\displaystyle \begin{array}{l}{\pi}_{0\mathrm{i}}={\gamma}_{00}+{\zeta}_{0\mathrm{i}}\left({\pi}_{0\mathrm{i}}={\gamma}_{00}++{\gamma}_{01}{\mathrm{TargetCal}}_{\mathrm{i}}+{\zeta}_{0\mathrm{i}}\mathrm{for}\ \mathrm{dinner}\right)\\ {}{\pi}_{1\mathrm{i}}={\gamma}_{10}\\ {}{\pi}_{2\mathrm{i}}={\gamma}_{20}\\ {}{\pi}_{3\mathrm{i}}={\gamma}_{30}\end{array}} $$

where Yij is each momentary calorie intake for the ith patient; Companyij is a corresponding accompanying person, recorded as alone or with someone; Placeij is the corresponding eating place; home, office/school, dining out, or other (e.g. outdoors); Stressij is the corresponding preceding psychological stress; TargetCali is the target calorie intake for the ith patient. γ00 is the average true value of mean calorie intake when all predictors are zero. π1i, π2i, and π3i are the individual i’s slopes representing the effect of an accompanying person(s), eating place(s), or preceding psychological stress on momentary calorie intake, respectively, and γ10, γ20, and γ30 are the average slopes. εij and ζ0i are residuals at each level. Including ζ0i in the equation means that the intercept was modeled as random, which suggests that intercept can vary across individuals. The second level 2 equation included no residual, which means that the effect of an accompanying person(s), eating place(s), or preceding psychological stress on momentary calorie intake was modeled as a fixed effect.

Preceding anxiety and dietary calorie intake

Preceding anxiety was significantly, negatively associated with dietary intake only for breakfast (Table 5). The final model is as follows:

Table 5 Effect of preceding anxiety on calorie intake, for breakfast

Level 1 equation:

$$ {\mathrm{Y}}_{\mathrm{ij}}={\uppi}_{0\mathrm{i}}+{\uppi}_{1\mathrm{i}}{\mathrm{Company}}_{\mathrm{ij}}+{\uppi}_{2\mathrm{i}}{\mathrm{Place}}_{\mathrm{ij}}+{\uppi}_{3\mathrm{i}}{\mathrm{Anxiety}}_{\mathrm{ij}}+{\upvarepsilon}_{\mathrm{ij}} $$

Level 2 equation:

$$ {\displaystyle \begin{array}{l}{\pi}_{0\mathrm{i}}={\gamma}_{00}+{\zeta}_{0\mathrm{i}}\\ {}{\pi}_{1\mathrm{i}}={\gamma}_{10}\\ {}{\pi}_{2\mathrm{i}}={\gamma}_{20}\\ {}{\pi}_{3\mathrm{i}}={\gamma}_{30}\end{array}} $$

where Yij is each momentary calorie intake for the ith patient; Companyij is a corresponding accompanying person, either alone or with someone; Placeij is the corresponding eating place, home, office/school, dining out, or other; Anxietyij is the corresponding preceding anxiety.

Preceding depressive mood and dietary calorie intake

Preceding depressive mood was significantly, negatively associated with dietary intake only for lunch (Table 6). The final model is as follows:

Table 6 Effect of preceding depressive mood on calorie intake, for lunch

Level 1 equation:

$$ {\mathrm{Y}}_{\mathrm{ij}}={\uppi}_{0\mathrm{i}}+{\uppi}_{1\mathrm{i}}{\mathrm{Company}}_{\mathrm{ij}}+{\uppi}_{2\mathrm{i}}{\mathrm{Place}}_{\mathrm{ij}}+{\uppi}_{3\mathrm{i}}{\mathrm{Depression}}_{\mathrm{ij}}+{\upvarepsilon}_{\mathrm{ij}} $$

Level 2 equation:

$$ {\displaystyle \begin{array}{l}{\pi}_{0\mathrm{i}}={\gamma}_{00}+{\zeta}_{0\mathrm{i}}\\ {}{\pi}_{1\mathrm{i}}={\gamma}_{10}\\ {}{\pi}_{2\mathrm{i}}={\gamma}_{20}\\ {}{\pi}_{3\mathrm{i}}={\gamma}_{30}\end{array}} $$

where Yij is each momentary calorie intake for the ith patient; Companyij is a corresponding accompanying person, either alone or with someone; Placeij is the corresponding eating place, home, office/school, dining out, or other; Depressionij is the corresponding preceding depressive mood.

Discussion

Using computerized EMA for 6 months, significant within-individual relationships between preceding psychological factors and calorie intake within 5 hours were shown in patients with type 2 diabetes. Although a causal relation could not be established, our results support the possibility that psychological factors can influence the calorie intake of patients with type 2 diabetes, which has not been investigated previously on a daily-life basis.

In the present study, the results showed differences in the association between preceding psychological factors and calorie intake among types of food. With regard to snacks, we showed that preceding psychological stress was positively associated with calorie intake. This result is consistent with the results of previous studies [3, 5, 6]. In the study by Lowe et al. [3], calorie intake from snacks increased when subjects with obesity were in a negative mood, although the study did not investigate the relation proportionally between negative mood and calorie intake. O’Connor et al. [5] reported a positive association between daily hassles and calorie intake from snacks, using only end-of-day assessments that did not include momentary psychological factors. Tomiyama et al. [6] used computerized EMA in a short two-day report that found preceding negative mood to be positively associated with calorie intake of snacks. Because the results of studies using such different methods were consistent, psychological stress would seem to increase calorie intake of snacks. Eating snacks may be a coping mechanism for reducing psychological stress [13].

With regard to regular meals, preceding psychological stress, anxiety, and depressive mood were negatively associated with calorie intake. Negative mood may cause not only emotional eating, but appetite loss mediated by autonomic reaction. Because the patients were following diet therapy advice, the calorie intake in their regular meals might have been affected by a physical reaction rather than by an emotional reaction. The results of previous studies were not consistent regarding the association between psychological factors and calorie intake. Lowe et al. [3] found no association between negative mood and the calorie intake from regular meals, while Patel et al. [4] reported that the calorie intake from regular meals was greater for “negative mood” than for “neutral mood”. Because all of the subjects were women in the two previous studies, it would be difficult to compare them to the results of the present study. Therefore, future studies will necessary to confirm the association between preceding psychological factors and regular meals.

When controlling for psychological factors, calorie intake was generally greater when eating with than without another person(s) and when eating out than when eating at home. This result supports the results shown in a previous study that calorie intake increased in social situations [4].

There were some limitations in the present study. First, the sample size was small. Second, most of the patients in the present study were men. Therefore, future studies with more patients including more men and women will be necessary. Also, there was no control group of healthy people, which makes it impossible to expand the results in the present study to healthy people. In addition, some of the dietary intake could be paired with preceding psychological factors, which might be subject to selection bias.

Conclusions

Preceding psychological factors influence the calorie intake of patients with type 2 diabetes. Better understanding these factors will be helpful for developing psychological interventions to prevent overeating.

Availability of data and materials

We are not able to share our data because sharing data is not permitted by our hospital or the ethics committee.

Abbreviations

AIC:

Akaike’s information criterion

EMA:

ecological momentary assessment

HbA1c:

hemoglobin A1c

PDA:

portable digital assistant

VAS:

visual analog scale

References

  1. Bonnet F, Irving K, Terra J, Nony P, Berthezène F, Moulin P. Anxiety and depression are associated with unhealthy lifestyle in patients at risk of cardiovascular disease. Atherosclerosis. 2005;178:339–44.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Stone AA, Shiffman SS. Ecological momentary assessment (EMA) in behavioral medicine. Ann Behav Med. 1994;16:199–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Lowe MR, Fisher EB. Emotional reactivity, emotional eating, and obesity: a naturalistic study. J Behav Med. 1983;6:135–49.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Patel KA, Schlundt DG. Impact of moods and social context on eating behavior. Appetite. 2001;36:111–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. O'Connor DB, Jones F, Conner M, McMillan B, Ferguson E. Effects of daily hassles and eating style on eating behavior. Health Psychol. 2008;27:S20–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Tomiyama AJ, Mann T, Comer L. Triggers of eating in everyday life. Appatite. 2009;52:72–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Stone AA, Shiffman S, Schwaltz JE, Broderick JE, Hufford MR. Patient non-compliance with paper diaries. BMJ. 2002;324:1192–3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Seino Y, Nanjo K, Tajima N, Kadowaki T, Kashiwagi A, Araki E, et al. Report of the committee on the classification and diagnostic criteria of diabetes mellitus: the Committee of the Japan Diabetes Society on the diagnostic criteria of diabetes mellitus. Diabetol Int. 2010;1:2–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Inada S, Yoshiuchi K, Iizuka Y, Ohashi K, Kikuchi H, Yamamoto Y, et al. Pilot study for the development of a self-care system for type 2 diabetes patients using a personal digital assistant (PDA). Int J Behav Med. 2016;23:295–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Fukuo W, Yoshiuchi K, Ohashi K, Togashi H, Sekine R, Kikuchi H, et al. Development of a hand-held personal digital assistant-based food diary with food photographs for Japanese subjects. J Am Diet Assoc. 2009;109:1232–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Kikuchi H, Yoshiuchi K, Miyasaka N, Ohashi K, Yamamoto Y, Kumano H, et al. Reliability of recalled self-report on headache intensity: investigation using ecological momentary assessment technique. Cephalalgia. 2006;26:1335–43.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Kikuchi H, Yoshiuchi K, Ando T, Yamamoto Y. Influence of psychological factors on acute exacerbation of tension-type headache: investigation by ecological momentary assessment. J Psychosom Res. 2015;79:239–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Macht M. How emotions affect eating: a five-way model. Appetite. 2008;50:1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank all of the study participants. Gratitude is also expressed to Dr. Kojima from Sharp Corporation for his technical help in developing the software in Widows Mobile.

Funding

The University of Tokyo (person in charge: Dr. Yoshiuchi) conducted cooperative research with Sharp Cooperation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

SI designed the study, analyzed the data, performed the statistical analysis, interpreted the results, and drafted the manuscript. YI and KO helped design the study and collect data. HK, YY and TK helped design the study. KY conceived of the present idea and helped design the study, analyze the data, interpret the results, and draft the manuscript. All authors have read, modified and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kazuhiro Yoshiuchi.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. The study protocol was approved by the ethics committee of the Graduate School of Medicine, The University of Tokyo (ID: 2728). Informed consent was provided by all individual participants included in the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

Author Yoshiuchi K. received research grants from Sharp Cooperation for the submitted work.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Inada, S., Iizuka, Y., Ohashi, K. et al. Preceding psychological factors and calorie intake in patients with type 2 diabetes: investigation by ecological momentary assessment. BioPsychoSocial Med 13, 20 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13030-019-0161-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13030-019-0161-4

Keywords