Skip to main content

Relationship between gender role, anger expression, thermal discomfort and sleep onset latency in women

Abstract

Background

Women with thermal discomfort from cold extremities (hands and feet; TDCE) often suffer from prolonged sleep onset latency (SOL). Suppressed anger could contribute to the genesis of both TDCE and prolonged SOL. The aim of the study was to test the hypothesis whether stereotypic feminine gender socialization (SFGS) is related to anger suppression (experienced anger inwards, Anger-In), which in turn could affect TDCE and SOL.

Methods

148 women, a sub-sample of a larger survey carried out in the Canton Basel-Stadt (Switzerland), sent back detailed postal questionnaires about SOL, TDCE, anger expression (STAXI, state -trait -anger -expression -inventory) and SFGS using a gender power inventory, estimating the degree of gender specific power expression explicitly within women by stereotypic feminine or male attribution. Statistics was performed by path analysis.

Results

A significant direct path was found from stereotypic feminine attribution to Anger-In and prolonged SOL. Additionally, a further indirect path from Anger-In via TDCE to SOL was found. In contrast, stereotypic male attribution was not related to Anger-In but was significantly associated with outwardly expressed anger.

Limitations

Self-reported data, retrospective cross-sectional survey, prospective studies are required including physiological measurements.

Conclusion

Stereotypic feminine gender socialization may play an important determinant for anger suppression, which subsequently can lead to thermal discomfort from cold extremities and prolonged sleep onset latency.

Background

It is well known that sleep onset latency (SOL) is prolonged in many life circumstances, such as stress, diverse illnesses and exciting behaviors before sleep [1]. Our extremities, hands and feet, are highly sensitive to react with vasoconstriction in these situations [2]. Both phenomenons seem to be closely interrelated. It has recently been published that subjects with prolonged SOL often suffer from thermal discomfort from cold extremities (TDCE) [3]. About 30% of women aged 20-40 years complain from cold hands and feet, but only 7% of men [3]. Independent of gender, each seventh subject complaining of TDCE also exhibits difficulties initiating sleep [3] - the relative risk in these subjects was approximately doubled.

TDCE is closely related to the body's thermophysiology, which can be declared as a state of increased body heat retention [3, 4]. Our sleep is typically initiated by body heat loss usually occurring on the declining portion of the CBT curve when its rate of change, and body heat loss, is maximal [5, 6]. Before lights off, body heat loss promotes sleepiness and the rapid onset of sleep via distal vasodilatation [3, 7, 8]. Distal vasodilatation and heat redistribution from the thermal core to the thermal shell seems to represent the main determining thermophysiological components of a well-orchestrated down-regulation of core body temperature in the evening [3]. Distal vasoconstriction and TDCE may therefore have clinical relevance for insomnia, in particular for difficulties initiating sleep.

Besides the many known determinants of TDCE [9], the skin sympathetic nervous system is of importance for vascular reactivity to cold and emotional stimuli [2]. Especially feeling of anger is related to increased vascular reactions [1016]. These findings suggest that feeling of anger could contribute to the etiology of the TDCE and hence of prolonged SOL. Furthermore, clinical and epidemiological studies have shown that girls are socialized in such a way that their expression of anger and aggression are strongly constrained [17, 18]. This socially conform behavior of women could lead to an anger suppression problem (e.g. higher STAXI Anger-In -score; [19, 20]. However, there is no study differentiating between women with or without a socially conform feminine behavior with respect to anger suppression and the development of psychosomatic disturbances such as TDCE and prolonged SOL.

Therefore, the aim of the study is to test the hypothesis whether stereotypic feminine gender socialization is related to anger suppression, which in turn could lead to TDCE and prolonged SOL, relying on a path-analysis with an a-priori formulated structure model. A part of the study results has been published in abstract form [21].

Methods

Subjects & Procedure

This study is a follow-up study of a large postal questionnaire survey on the relationship between sleep onset behavior and thermal discomfort [3], approved by the ethical committee of the cantons Basel-Stadt and Baselland (EKBB) and carried out between February and May 2006. The survey included 1400 men and 1400 women, aged 20-40 years, a random sample of the population register of Basel-Stadt, Switzerland. Based on the survey's finding that the prevalence of TDCE is 4 to 5 times higher in women than in men [3], a detailed postal questionnaire were sent to those women, who had consented to be re-contacted for a further study (N = 520 of total 1001 women). 148 of the 520 women returned completed questionnaires. They did not statistically differ from the rest of women who participated only in the first study part (N = 853) with respect to their socio- and bio-demographic characteristics shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Socio, psycho and bio-demographic characteristics

Instruments

Gender- Power- Inventory

Many attributes are clearly assigned to gender [22]. Certain assignments imply additionally a gender specific suppression or expression of power. Previous instruments addressed stereotypic feminine gender socialization [22, 23] but did not take into account the important aspect of power suppression or expression implicitly represented in certain attributes. For the present study, a new gender power inventory has been developed (by M.v.A.) estimating the degree of gender specific power suppression and expression explicitly within women. Power suppression can be estimated by specific stereotypic feminine attributes (SFA) and conversely power expression by specific stereotypic male attributes (SMA). Based on this background, twenty-one bipolar items have been chosen, ten items referring to specific power suppression (SFA; "in comparison to other women I'm more.: sensitive, controlled, adjusted, holding back, inhibited, avoiding confrontation, emotional, inferior, anxious, depressed"), and a further eleven items referring to specific power expression (SMA; "in comparison to other women I'm more.: loud, active, vigorous, resolute, impulsive, confident, dominant, forceful, spontaneous, aggressive, quick to make decision) [2227]. Four answer categories were given: absolutely not the case = 1, rather not the case = 2, rather the case = 3, absolutely the case = 4. A mean SFA and SMA -score has been calculated. With respect to other women, a high SFA -score indicates relative high power suppression and a high SMA -score indicates relative high power expression.

State-Trait-Anger-eXpression-Inventory (STAXI)

The STAXI [19, 20] was used for dispositional state and trait anger, as well as for anger expression. It consists of three different scales, State Anger (10 items), Trait Anger (10 items), and anger expression (24 items). SA refers to the intensity of the individual's angry feelings at the time of testing. TA measures the extent to which an individual is predisposed to experience anger or frustration in a range of situations. Individuals are asked to indicate on a four-point scale how often they generally react or behave in the manner described by each item. Anger expression consists of three subscales. Anger-In measures the extent to which people hold things in or suppress anger when they are angry or furious. Anger-Out describes the extent to which a person expresses her emotional experience of anger in an outwardly manner. Anger-Control involves expenditure of energy to monitor and control the physical or verbal expression of anger. A high score on each of these scales represents a high tendency or frequency to express that mode of anger. The STAXI has demonstrated good internal reliability and validity based on results from a variety of samples and cultures [19, 20].

Thermal discomfort from cold extremities (TDCE)

Two questions referring to the leading symptoms of TDCE were used for its definition: 1. In the past month, how intensively did you suffer from cold hands? 2. In the past month, how intensively did you suffer from cold feet? Answer categories: 0 = 'not at all', 1 = 'a little', 2 = 'quite', 3 = 'extraordinary'. For dimensional analysis a TDCE-score of the two questions was calculated (Σ = 0-6). TDCE has been externally validated with fingertip skin temperature [3].

Sleep parameters

Sleep onset latency (SOL) was inquired by the question: "During the past month, how long (in minutes) has it usually taken to fall asleep?" Log transformed sleep onset latency [log(SOL)] was utilized for dimensional analysis to obtain normally distributed values. Time awake and number of waking-ups during a night's sleep episode were asked by the questions "In the past month, for how long have you been awake during a night's sleep episode?" and "In the past month, how often did you usually wake-up during a night's sleep episode?", respectively. Sleep times for the night's sleep episode were asked in a similar way e.g.: "In the past month when did you usually switch the lights off?"

Statistical analyses

Statistical analysis was performed using Statistica 7.0 (StatSoft, Inc., Tulsa, OK 74104, USA). Pearson's correlation analysis was used for bivariate testing. Path analysis was calculated using the statistical package AMOS 5.0.

Results

Sample characteristics

Table 1 shows the characteristic of the study sample. Our sub-sample of 148 women didn't statistically differ from women of the large survey carried out in Canton Basel-Stadt [3] with respect to BMI, age, contraceptive prevalence, sleep times, sleep onset latency, hours work/week, number of cigarette consumption and TDCE, indicating a representative sample of the general population of Basel-Stadt.

Bivariate correlation analysis

Pairwise correlations of the relevant variables are shown in Table 2. SFA is highly negatively correlated with SMA. SFA exhibited positive correlations with Anger-Control and most remarkable and highly significant with AI. SFA also correlated significantly with TDCE and log (SOL), whereas TDCE is positively inter-correlated with log (SOL). SMA is significantly correlated with all three anger expression scores, negatively with Anger-In and Anger-Control and positively with Anger-Out. SMA is negatively correlated with log (SOL). Trait and state anger did not exhibit significant correlations with SFA, SMA, TDCE and log (SOL) (data therefore not shown).

Table 2 Inter-correlations (r) of selected variables

Interestingly enough, time awake during the sleep episode (awake in min) neither correlated with any of the anger expression scores nor with SFA and SMA. The three anger expression scores show similar significant inter-correlations as previously published [19, 20], confirming the reliable factor structure of the questionnaire in our sample. Among the anger expression scores only Anger-In correlated with log (SOL) (r = 0.322). Furthermore, log (SOL) showed a significant correlation (r = 0.329) with time awake during the sleep episode indicating that sleep onset disturbances are associated with sleep maintenance disturbance.

In order to highlight the relationship between anger expression and stereotypic gender attribution, SFA and SMA has been analyzed as a function of high or low level (median splitted -data) of Anger-In, Anger-Control and Anger-Out -scores (Figure 1). Women with high Anger-In score (>median) rated significantly higher SFA than those with low Anger-In score (≤median), but did not differ in SMA. Conversely, women with high Anger-Out -score rated significantly higher SMA than those with low Anger-Out -score, but did not differ in SFA. Anger-Control differentiated regarding to both SFA and SMA: in women with high Anger-Control -score, significantly higher SFA and lower SMA was found, respectively.

Figure 1
figure 1

Stereotypic masculine (upper panel, SMA) or feminine (lower panel, SFA) attributes according to high (beyond median, black column) or low (equal or below median, white column) anger expression categories. AI = anger inwards, AO = anger out, AC = anger control. * = p < 0.05; ** = p < 0.01 indicate significant differences between median split values (> vs. ≤ median) within AI, AO and AC.

Path-analysis

To test how the variables combine together, a structure model was formulated with SOL as a purely endogenous variable, Anger-In and TDCE as partially endogenous variables and SFA and SMA as exogenous variables. Significant pathways (p < 0.05, indicated by thick arrows) are shown in Figure 2 with standardized path coefficients (partial regression coefficients) beside the path arrows. Such path coefficients are interpreted as in multiple regression models, thus controlling for the influence of other prior variables. The obtained model was recursive, over identified, and fitted the data well as shown by the different fit indices. Thus the chi-square fit index (Chi square = 5.151, df = 5, p = 0.398) clearly did not reach significance, which is a desirable result indicating that the model fit the data well. The three indices CFI (0.998), TLI (0.995) and RMSEA (0.014) indicated a very good fit. As assumed, there were in fact significant paths from SFA to Anger-In to TDCE to SOL. However, there is an additional direct path from Anger-In to SOL. No significant direct paths from SFA or SMA to SOL, and to TDCE were found and were therefore set to 0 in the model. When replacing SOL by 'minutes awake during nocturnal sleep' (an indicator for sleep maintenance), none of the two path coefficients leading to this new purely endogenous variable were significant, thus underlining the selectivity of our finding -- SOL seems to be sensitive to the influence of Anger-In, SFGS and TDCE.

Figure 2
figure 2

Path diagram showing the relation between stereotypic feminine (SFA) and masculine (SMA) attribution, Anger-In, thermal discomfort from cold extremities (TDCE) and sleep onset latency [SOL, log transformed SOL (log(SOL)]. Thick arrows indicate significant pathways (for details see text). Chi2 = 5.151, df = 5 p = 0.398; CFI = 0.998; TLI = 0.995; RMSEA = 0.014; Pclose = 0.596.

Discussion

The study shows that stereotypic feminine gender socialization, as indicated by high power-suppression, may constitute an important determinant for turning experienced anger inwards and subsequently leading to thermal discomfort with cold extremities and prolonged sleep onset latency.

From a sociological point of view, women can't be powerful because of their gender role - powerful women lose their femininity as it is defined and requested from society [28], p.143. In general, they occupy subordinate status in patriarchal societies that reserve power for men [29]. However, men may not necessarily get angrier than women. When women, and men, get angry it reflects a process of gender socialization, more specifically, how men and women have learned to cope with anger [27]. The high pressure to be a conformed woman in society can easily be observed in many behaviors as expressed in the widespread wearing of uncomfortable clothing and in typical feminine body positions (so-called gendering or doing gender, e.g. oblique head position, an attitude to submission and servility [30]), which do not allow expression of power.

In our sample we could show that women who rated themselves as more stereotypic feminine in comparison to other women (i.e. being less powerful) exhibit significantly higher anger suppression. Figure 1 depicts a kind of internal validation of gender power inventory using the validated instrument STAXI [19]. Women with high Anger-In rated significantly higher SFA than those with low Anger-In and conversely women with high Anger-Out rated higher SMA than those with low Anger-Out. Furthermore, gender differences in anger expression have to be expected from the obvious reality in the society. Diverse studies, including e.g. crime statistics [31], confirm that men as a sex are more physically and verbally aggressive than women. However, many studies using the validated instrument STAXI found no, or only small, gender differences in experiencing and expression of anger [19]. One reason for this discrepancy could be that women and men compare themselves within their own gender, abolishing therefore real existing differences. In other words, when the STAXI questionnaire would be filled out explicitly in comparison to the other gender, a clear gender difference can be expected (i.e. higher Anger-In in women and high Anger-Out in men).

The present study has limitations. First, it is a cross-sectional investigation and thus cannot causally assess the direction of the associations between the different dimensions (SFA, Anger-In, TDCE and SOL). To reveal a direct impact of e.g. SFA on Anger-In, prospective evaluations are required across different cultures and various age groups. Nevertheless, our analysis has confirmed what we a-priori hypothesized. Second, the study was based on self-reported information, which should be based on known validity and reliability of the defined scores. However, the chosen items for SFA and SMA loaded with low and high sex stereotype index scores of a former study using questionnaires concerning stereotypic gender socialization [22], respectively. This may at least indicate some external validity at least with respect to male or female associated sex-stereotype adjectives. Nevertheless, further validation studies are needed for SFA and SMA, which is presently under investigation. Additionally, in a separate sample of 25 women, we found good test-retest reliability for SFA and SMA over a two-week interval (r = 0.865 and r = 0.946, respectively). Internal consistency of the two scores has also been shown in the present study sample with a high Cronbach's alpha of 0.721 for the 10 SFA -items and 0.819 for the 11 SMA -items.

Moreover, for both physiological related measures (TDCE and SOL) external validation studies already exist. TDCE has been objectively validated by finger skin temperature measures [3] and SOL by polysomnographic recordings [32]. Additionally, ambulatory and controlled laboratory studies have shown that women with TDCE and prolonged SOL do, in fact, exhibit lower distal skin temperatures and longer SOL than controls [33, 34]. It is well known that these physiological measures are strongly influenced by the autonomic nervous system (i.e. the sympathetic nerve activity) [2]. Chronic primary insomnia has been characterized as a state of hyperarousal seen, for example, in higher sympathetic nervous activity as measured by spectral analysis of heart rate variability [1, 35]. We have recently shown that women with TDCE and difficulties initiating sleep exhibit a higher sympathetic/parasympathetic ratio found by heart rate variability analysis [36]. It can be assumed that activation of the sympathetic nervous system represents a biological pathway transforming anger suppression into manifested physiological changes such as TDCE and prolonged SOL. A recent study supports that anger suppression, and not outwardly expressed anger, is the significant determinant for somatoform disorders [37]. Our study suggests that sociocultural aspects, such as stereotypic gender socialization, have to be considered. It is well known that expression of anger and its somatization exhibit cultural differences. For instance, TDCE is even more prevalent in Japan (where it is called hi-e-sho, meaning 'cold syndrome' or 'vasospastic syndrome') than in European countries [9, 38]. Taken together, in order to explain the chain of anger suppression to TDCE and prolonged SOL in women, SFGS has to be taken into account.

Conclusion

Our findings suggest that stereotypic feminine gender socialization may play an important determinant for anger suppression, which subsequently can lead to thermal discomfort from cold extremities and prolonged sleep onset latency.

Abbreviations

AC:

controlled anger expression

AI:

anger expression inwards

AO:

outwardly expressed anger

BMI:

body mass index

CFI:

comparative fit index

log (SOL):

log transformed sleep onset latency

Pclose:

p-value testing the null hypothesis that RMSEA is no greater than .05

RMSEA:

root mean square error of approximation

SOL:

sleep onset latency

SFGS:

stereotypic feminine gender socialization

SFA:

stereotypic feminine attributes

SMA:

stereotypic male attributes

STAXI:

state-trait-anger-expression-inventory

TDCE:

thermal discomfort with cold extremities

TLI:

Tucker-Lewis index

References

  1. Roth T, Roehrs T, Pies R: Insomnia: pathophysiology and implications for treatment. Sleep Med Rev. 2007, 11: 71-79. 10.1016/j.smrv.2006.06.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Kistler A, Mariauzouls C, von Berlepsch K: Fingertip temperture as an indicator for sympathetic responses. Int J Psychophysiol. 1998, 29: 35-41. 10.1016/S0167-8760(97)00087-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Kräuchi K, Fontana Gasio P, Vollenweider S, Von Arb M, Dubler B, Orgül S, Flammer J, Zemp Stutz E: Cold extremities and difficulties initiating sleep: Evidence of co-morbitity from a random sample of a Swiss urban population. J Sleep Res. 2008, 26: 26-38.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Kräuchi K: The thermophysiological cascade leading to sleep initiation in relation to phase of entrainment. Sleep Med Rev. 2007, 11: 439-451. 10.1016/j.smrv.2007.07.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Campbell SS, Broughton RJ: Rapid decline in body temperature before sleep: fluffing the physiological pillow?. Chronobiol Int. 1994, 11: 126-131. 10.3109/07420529409055899.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Kräuchi K, Cajochen C, Werth E, Wirz-Justice A: Functional link between distal vasodilation and sleep-onset latency?. Am J Physiol. 2000, 278: R741-R748.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Magnusson G: On narcolepsy II. Studies on diurnal variation in the skin temperatures in narcoleptics. Acta Psychiatr Neurol. 1943, 18: 457-485. 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1943.tb09286.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Kräuchi K, Cajochen C, Werth E, Wirz-Justice A: Warm feet promote the rapid onset of sleep. Nature. 1999, 401: 36-37. 10.1038/43366.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Flammer J, Pache M, Resink T: Vasospasm, its role in the pathogenesis of diseases with particular reference to the eye. Prog Retin Eye Res. 2001, 20: 319-349. 10.1016/S1350-9462(00)00028-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Mittelmann B, Wolff HG: Emotions and skin temperature. Observations on patients during psychotherapeutic (psychoanalytic) interviews. 1943, 5: 211-231.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Faber SD, Burns JW: Anger management style, degree of expressed anger, and gender influence cardiovascular recovery from interpersonal harassment. J Behav Med. 1996, 19: 31-53. 10.1007/BF01858173.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Miller SB: Cardiovascular reactivity in anger-defensive individuals: the influence of task demands. Psychosom Med. 1993, 55: 78-85.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Siegman AW: Cardiovascular consequences of expressing, experiencing, and repressing anger. J Behav Med. 1993, 16: 539-569. 10.1007/BF00844719.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Siegman AW, Snow SC: The outward expression of anger, the inward experience of anger and CVR: the role of vocal expression. J Behav Med. 1997, 20: 29-45. 10.1023/A:1025535129121.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Vogele C, Jarvis A, Cheeseman K: Anger suppression, reactivity, and hypertension risk: gender makes a difference. Ann Behav Med. 1997, 19: 61-69. 10.1007/BF02883428.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Vogele C, Steptoe A: Emotional coping and tonic blood pressure as determinants of cardiovascular responses to mental stress. J Hypertens. 1992, 10: 1079-1087.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Gornick V, Moran B, Eds: Women in Sexist Society. 1971, New York: Basic Books

  18. Kaplan A, Bean J, Eds: Beyond Sex-Role Stereotypes: Reading Toward a Psychology of Androgyny. 1976, Boston: Little, Brown

  19. Spielberger C, Ed: State-Trait-Anger-Expression-Inventory (STAXI). 1988, Odessa, FL Psychological Assessment Resources

  20. Schwenkmezger P, Hodapp V: A questionnaire for assessing anger and expression of anger. Z Klin Psychol Psychopathol Psychother. 1991, 39: 63-68.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. von Arb M, Gompper B, Fontana Gasio P, Vollenweider S, Orgül S, Flammer J, Zemp Stutz E, Kräuchi K: Women with vasospastic syndrome exhibit difficulties initiating sleep and turn their experienced anger inwards. Sleep. 2007, 30 (Abstr Suppl): A375-

    Google Scholar 

  22. Williams J, Best D: Measuring sex stereotypes: A multinational study. 1990, Newbury Park, CA: Sage

    Google Scholar 

  23. Alfermann D: Geschlechterrollen und geschlechtstypisches Verhalten. 1996, Stuttgart, Berlin, Köln: Kohlhammer

    Google Scholar 

  24. Mitscherlich M: Die friedfertige Frau. Eine psychoanalytische Untersuchung zur Aggression der Geschlechter. 1985, Frankfurt a.M.: Fischer

    Google Scholar 

  25. Cox DL, Bruckner KH, Stabb SD: The anger advantage. 2003, New York: Broadway/Doubleday

    Google Scholar 

  26. Bourdieu P: Die männliche Herrschaft. 2005, Frankfurt a.M.: Suhrkamp Verlag

    Google Scholar 

  27. Tangney JP, Hill-Barlow D, Wagner PE, Marschall DE, Borenstein JK, Sanftner J, Mohr T, Gramzow R: Assessing individual differences in constructive versus destructive responses to anger across the lifespan. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1996, 70: 780-796. 10.1037/0022-3514.70.4.780.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Goldhor Lerner H, Ed: Das missdeutete Geschlecht. 1991, Zürich: Kreuz Verlag

  29. Kakar S: Kindheit und Gesellschaft in Indien. Eine psychoanalytische Studie. 1988, Frankfurt a.M.: Nexus Verlag

    Google Scholar 

  30. Fenstermaker S, West C, Eds: Doing Gender, Doing Difference: Inequality, Power, and Institutional Change. 2002, New York: Routledge

  31. Greenfeld LA, Snell TL: Women Offenders. Statistics BoJ, Washington, DC. 1999, 283-316.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Silva GE, Goodwin JL, Sherrill DL, Arnold JL, Bootzin RR, Smith T, Walsleben JA, Baldwin CM, Quan SF: Relationship between reported and measured sleep times: the sleep heart health study (SHHS). J Clin Sleep Med. 2007, 3: 622-630.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Vollenweider S, Wirz-Justice A, Flammer J, Orgül S, Kräuchi K: Chronobiological characterization of women with primary vasospastic syndrome: body heat loss capacity in relation to sleep initiation and phase of entrainment. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2008, 294: R630-638.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Gompper B, Vollenweider S, Renz C, van Someren EJW, Wirz-Justice A, Orgül S, Flammer J, Kräuchi K: Ambulatory measurement of skin temperatures and the sleep-wake-cycle in women with vasospastic syndrome and controls. Sleep. 2007, 30 (Abstr Suppl): A51-A52.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Bonnet MH, Arand DL: Heart rate variability in insomniacs and matched normal sleepers. Psychosom Med. 1998, 60: 610-615.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Anders D, Vollenweider S, Hofstetter M, Wirz-Justice A, Orgül S, Flammer J, Kräuchi K: Women with difficulties initiating sleep and vasospastic syndrome exhibit lower heart rate variability in the high frequency band. Sleep. 2008, 31 (Suppl): A29-

    Google Scholar 

  37. Koh KB, Kim DK, Kim SY, Park JK, Han M: The relation between anger management style, mood and somatic symptoms in anxiety disorders and somatoform disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2008, 160: 372-379. 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.06.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Nagashima K, Yoda T, Yagishita T, Taniguchi A, Hosono T, Kanosue K: Thermal regulation and comfort during a mild-cold exposure in young japanese women complaining of unusual coldness. J Appl Physiol. 2002, 92: 1029-1035.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Our work has been supported by the Schwickert-Stiftung and the Swiss National Science Foundation Grant # 3200B0-116504) to KK. These founding sources had no involvement in the study design, collection, analysis and interpretation of the data, in writing the report, and in the decision to submit for publication.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kurt Kräuchi.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

MvA and KK conceived the study and wrote the manuscript. BG carried out the study and performed data processing. AHM and KK performed the data analysis. EZS, SO and JF provided advice on the study design and assisted in drafting the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Authors’ original file for figure 2

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

von Arb, M., Gompper, B., Meyer, A.H. et al. Relationship between gender role, anger expression, thermal discomfort and sleep onset latency in women. BioPsychoSocial Med 3, 11 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1186/1751-0759-3-11

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1751-0759-3-11

Keywords