Skip to main content

Clinical application of somatosensory amplification in psychosomatic medicine

Abstract

Many patients with somatoform disorders are frequently encountered in psychosomatic clinics as well as in primary care clinics. To assess such patients objectively, the concept of somatosensory amplification may be useful. Somatosensory amplification refers to the tendency to experience a somatic sensation as intense, noxious, and disturbing. It may have a role in a variety of medical conditions characterized by somatic symptoms that are disproportionate to demonstrable organ pathology. It may also explain some of the variability in somatic symptomatology found among different patients with the same serious medical disorder. It has been assessed with a self-report questionnaire, the Somatosensory Amplification Scale. This instrument was developed in a clinical setting in the U.S., and the reliability and validity of the Japanese and Turkish versions have been confirmed as well.

Many studies have attempted to clarify the specific role of somatosensory amplification as a pathogenic mechanism in somatization. It has been reported that somatosensory amplification does not correlate with heightened sensitivity to bodily sensations and that emotional reactivity exerts its influence on somatization via a negatively biased reporting style. According to our recent electroencephalographic study, somatosensory amplification appears to reflect some aspects of long-latency cognitive processing rather than short-latency interoceptive sensitivity.

The concept of somatosensory amplification can be useful as an indicator of somatization in the therapy of a broad range of disorders, from impaired self-awareness to various psychiatric disorders. It also provides useful information for choosing appropriate pharmacological or psychological therapy. While somatosensory amplification has a role in the presentation of somatic symptoms, it is closely associated with other factors, namely, anxiety, depression, and alexithymia that may also influence the same. The specific role of somatosensory amplification with regard to both neurological and psychological function should be clarified in future studies. In this paper, we will explain the concept of amplification and describe its role in psychosomatic illness.

Assessment of stress-related conditions

Stress is the term used to define the body's physiological and/or psychological reaction to circumstances that require behavioral adjustment. According to the Japanese National Survey of Health in 2004 [1], 49% of those 12 years or older reported experiencing stress in their daily lives. In this survey, the subjects answered "yes" if they perceived stress in any of 28 domains including work, family and neighborhood relations as well as living-, social-, financial-, and health-related situations. A higher percentage of perceived stress was observed in women (53%) than in men (45%); the percentage of perceived stress has continued to increase over the years. in both sexes. Work-related problems were the most frequent stressors, followed by health-related and then financial problems [1]. One of the interesting findings of this national survey[1] was that stress was more frequently reported by those complaining of any physical or psychological symptoms; 69% of 37 million people with such symptoms reported stress as opposed to only 39% of 75 million people without symptoms who did (p < 0.0001, chi-square test). These results [1] suggest that those perceiving psychosocial stress are also likely to complain of mind/body symptoms.

The symptoms related to psychosocial stress are often temporary and disappear with the relief of such stress. However, a specific illness may be caused when the experienced stressors are too intense and persistent. When people are vulnerable to stress because of their character and ability to adapt, a psychosomatic illness is likely to occur even if the stressors are mild or moderate[2]. The Japanese Society of Psychosomatic Medicine defines psychosomatic illness as any physical condition with organic or functional damage affected by psychosocial factors in its onset or development[3]. This definition largely corresponds to that of "psychosocial factors affecting general medical conditions (code 316.00)" of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders fourth edition, text revision (DSM-IV-TR) [4], published by the American Psychiatric Association.

Somatization and psychosomatic illness

According to a study[5] of outpatients visiting a Japanese psychosomatic clinic (n = 1,432), the most common physical disorders observed were autonomic nervous dysfunction, irritable bowel syndrome, essential hypertension, and hyperventilation. Eating disorders, anxiety disorders, and depressive episodes were also prevalent. When the DSM-III-R or DSM-IV criteria were applied to the total sample, "somatoform disorders not otherwise specified" became the most common diagnosis, followed by bulimia nervosa, depressive disorders not otherwise specified, anorexia nervosa, conversion disorder, major depression or depressive disorder, panic disorder with agoraphobia, and psychological factors affecting physical (or medical) condition.

These findings appear to conflict with those from Western countries[6, 7]. For example, a study in an Italian psychosomatic clinic [6] showed that the most frequent diagnosis was "psychological factors affecting physical condition," followed by affective illness, anxiety disturbance, and somatoform disorders according to the DSM-III criteria. In a Japanese study[5], a detailed manual of diagnoses was made, and the physicians specializing in psychosomatic medicine discussed the patients' diagnoses in order to improve the reliability of diagnoses; however, many patients were still categorized into "somatoform disorders not otherwise specified." These studies indicate that there is considerable confusion and ambiguity in diagnosing patients with somatization. To assess such patients more objectively, the concept of somatosensory amplification may be useful in clinical practices.

Concept of somatosensory amplification

Somatosensory amplification refers to the tendency to experience a somatic sensation as intense, noxious, and disturbing [8]. The construct of somatosensory amplification is helpful in the assessment of somatization and in the conceptualization of psychosomatic illness [8–10]. Somatosensory amplification may have a role in a variety of medical conditions characterized by somatic symptoms that are disproportionate to demonstrable organ pathology. It may also explain some of the variability in somatic symptomatology found among different patients with the same serious nonpsychiatric medical disorder.

Studies of amplification in patients with somatoform disorders have been conducted. These studies have resulted in the standardization of the Somatosensory Amplification Scale (SSAS) checklist in 1990 [11]. (Table 1) The original SSAS[11] was developed in a clinical setting in the U.S., and the reliability and validity of the Japanese[12] and Turkish forms[13] of the SSAS have been confirmed as well. It is a 10-item self-report questionnaire, and the respondents rate the degree to which each statement is ''characteristic of you in general,'' on an ordinal scale of 1 to 5. A higher total score indicates greater symptom amplification (score range of 10 to 50).

Table 1 Somatosensory Amplification Scale

According to our clinical experiences and previous studies targeting the Japanese population, SSAS scores over 30 may reflect a highly somatizing condition; the average SSAS scores were 24–29 in groups of university students[14], office workers[15], and outpatients visiting a general internal medicine clinic [10] whereas it was 32 in the patients visiting a psychosomatic clinic[10]. Based on such experimental and epidemiological studies, we believe that somatosensory amplification appears to have both trait-like and state-like properties[10, 14, 15].

The SSAS is useful in briefly and objectively evaluating patients with mind/body distress. The total number of reported somatic symptoms has been considered to be a powerful predictor of functional impairment in physical, psychological, and social functioning [16], and the SSAS scores were shown to be closely associated with the total number of somatic symptoms in patients visiting a psychosomatic clinic[10].

Somatosensory amplification and alexithymia

Alexithymia is a personality construct derived from the clinical observation of patients with psychosomatic illness [17]. It is characterized by difficulty in distinguishing between emotions and bodily sensations, difficulty identifying and describing emotions, and a mechanistic, concrete, literal cognitive style. Evidence has suggested that alexithymia is associated with a tendency to develop functional somatic symptoms [18–20]. Our recent study reported that the SSAS was significantly correlated with a Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS), in the sample of individuals with psychosomatic illness[10]. High rates of alexithymia have been reported in patients with essential hypertension, myocardial infarction, inflammatory bowel diseases, functional gastrointestinal disorders, and chronic pain [21], and the close relationship between alexithymia and somatosensory amplification has been demonstrated in chronic pain [22] and functional dyspepsia[23]. The statistical and clinical association between somatosensory amplification and alexithymic characteristics appears logical. The roles of these two psychological concepts in clinical conditions should be further studied to clarify symptom generation and perception in patients with psychosomatic illness.

Role of somatosensory amplification

Three components of somatosensory amplification have been described[24]: bodily hypervigilance that involves heightened self-scrutiny and increased attention to unpleasant bodily sensations; the tendency to select and focus on certain relatively weak or infrequent sensations; and the tendency to appraise ambiguous or vague visceral and somatic sensations as abnormal, pathological, and symptomatic of disease, rather than considering them to be normal. This cognitive appraisal causes alarm and anxiety in relation to the perception of symptoms and is proposed to act as the intermediary between the perception of bodily sensations on one hand and hypochondriacal beliefs and behaviors on the other. Many studies have attempted to clarify the specific role of somatosensory amplification as a pathogenic mechanism in somatization [25–27]. A recent study[28] failed to find a significant relationship between the SSAS and heartbeat detection ability and interoceptive sensitivity, suggesting that self-reported somatosensory amplification does not correlate with objectively measured sensitivity to bodily sensations. In another study[29], emotional reactivity appeared to exert its influence on somatization via a negatively biased reporting style and not via somatic sensitivity.

To elucidate the link between somatosensory amplification and sensitivity to bodily sensations, we conducted an electroencephalographic (EEG)[14] study in 33 university students examining the relationship between somatosensory amplification and four different types of evoked potentials, i.e., short-latency somatosensory evoked potential (SSEP), brainstem auditory-evoked potential (BAEP), visual evoked potential (VEP), and auditory event-related potential (ERP). (Figure 1) We found that the SSAS was significantly associated with the parameters of auditory ERP (i.e., the P200 latency and P300 amplitude) after adjusting for the effects of the TAS and of the depression and tension-anxiety subscales of the Profile of Mood States[14].(Table 2) This significant relationship between the SSAS and auditory ERP appears important. The SSEP (normally 8.0–30.0 ms in latency) reflects mechanical processing in short pathways from sensory-organ to the primary cortex, whereas auditory ERP (normally 100–350 ms in latency) reflects cognitive processing of bodily sensations which they operationally define as processing in long pathways from the sensory-organ to cerebral cortex via complex synaptic circuits[30, 31]. Based on the insignificant findings of SSEP in the study[14], the SSAS was not suggested to be a measure of mechanical conduction from the sensory organs to the first sensory cortex areas. Rather the SSAS seems to be more closely related to the processing of sensory input at higher level of central nervous system. Auditory ERP is divided into early- (<100 ms) and late- (>100 ms) occurring components [30, 31]. The late component represents aspects of information processing, such as attention allocation and activation of immediate memory, while the early component represents the activity of the sensory nerves, brainstem, and primary sensory cortex. Thus a delayed P200 and diminished P300 amplitude may reflect a disturbance in the awareness of or the attention paid to afferent stimuli due to abnormally increased levels of physiological inhibition, possibly at the level of the brainstem, cortex, or both [32, 33]. Although the findings [14] should be viewed as preliminary, somatosensory amplification appears to reflect some aspects of long-latency cognitive processing rather than short-latency interoceptive sensitivity from the viewpoint of EEG.

Table 2 Evoked potentials associated with the SSAS
Figure 1
figure 1

Short-latency somatosensory evoked potential (SSEP), brainstem auditory-evoked potential (BAEP), visual evoked potential (VEP), and auditory event-related potential (ERP) recorded in human.

Use of SSAS

The concept of somatosensory amplification enables us to quantify the intensity of various somatic symptoms that patients complain about, eliminating the subjective judgment of physicians. The objective measurement of somatosensory sensitivity with a psychophysiological instrument is difficult and time consuming. The SSAS is simple and requires less than 10 min to complete. Although there are many reliable questionnaires for assessing somatoform symptoms, such as the Whitely Index, Somatic Symptom Inventory, the hypochondriasis subscale on the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory, and the somatization scale on the Symptom Checklist 90R [34–37], the SSAS enables the evaluation of somatosensory amplification in various diseases with fewer questions.

The SSAS should be used in combination with other psychological questionnaires in a test battery. This is because mood states, psychosocial stress, and the number of somatic symptoms can all influence somatosensory amplification as shown previously[10]. The choice of additional instruments will vary depending on the study aims; however, at least mood states and the severity of somatic impairment should be evaluated.

The SSAS can be useful as an indicator of somatization in the therapy of a broad range of disorders, from impaired self-awareness [37–39] to various psychiatric disorders[40, 41]. The concept of somatosensory amplification helps patients and physicians to better understand situations in which the psychiatric symptoms do not match the patients' clinical conditions and also provide useful information for choosing the appropriate pharmacological or psychological therapy. The SSAS would be useful in the treatment of patients with specific psychosomatic illness (e.g., irritable bowel syndrome[37, 42, 43] and chronic pain[22, 44–46]), psychiatric disorders (e.g., somatoform disorders [47–52], anxiety disorders[53, 54], and mood disorders[50, 55]), stress reaction (e.g., reaction to bereavement[56] and other important psychosocial events[55]), and medical disorders (e.g., infectious disease[56] and heart disease[57, 58]).

Conclusion

A total of 50 English-language articles[8, 10, 11, 13–15, 20, 22–29, 33–55, 57–68] were identified using the text words "somatosensory amplification" through a MEDLINE search from 1966 to April 2007. Somatization is a common feature in patients with mind/body distress, and the concept of somatosensory amplification provides a new approach to psychosomatic research [69–71]. It can help us to identify the explicit factors mediating the links between somatic and psychological symptoms. While somatosensory amplification has a role in the presentation of somatic symptoms, it is closely associated with other factors, namely, anxiety, depression, and alexithymia that may also influence the same. The specific role of somatosensory amplification with regard to both neurological and psychological function should be clarified in future studies.

Abbreviations

BAEP:

Bainstem auditory-evoked potential.

DSM-III:

Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, third edition.

DSM-III-R:

Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, third edition, revised.

DSM-IV:

Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, fourth edition.

DSM-IV-TR:

Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, fourth edition, text revision.

EEG:

Electroencephalography.

ERP:

Event-related potential.

SD:

Standard deviation.

SSAS:

Somatosensory amplification scale.

SSEP:

Short-latency somatosensory evoked potential.

TAS:

Tronto Alexithymia Scale

VEP:

Visual evoked potential.

References

  1. Japanese Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare: National Survey of Health 2004. 2006, Tokyo: Kosei Toukei Kyoukai, in Japanese

    Google Scholar 

  2. Nakao M: Mind/body medicine and stress management. J Med Saf. 2005, 2: 17-24.

    Google Scholar 

  3. The Committee of Education and Training of the Japanese Society of Psychosomatic Medicine: An updated treatment guideline of psychosomatic medicine. Jpn J Psychosom Med. 1991, 31: 537-576. in Japanese

    Google Scholar 

  4. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, fourth edition, text revision. 2000, Washington D.C: American Psychiatric Press

    Book  Google Scholar 

  5. Nakao M, Nomura S, Yamanaka G, Kumano H, Kuboki T: Assessment of patients by DSMIIIR and DSMIV in a Japanese Psychosomatic Clinic. Psychother Psychosom. 1998, 67: 43-49. 10.1159/000012258.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Fava GA, Freyberger HJ, Bech P, Christodoulou G, Sensky T, Theorell T, Wise TN: Diagnostic criteria for use in psychosomatic research. Psychother Psychosom. 1995, 63: 1-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Fava GA, Fabbri S, Sirri L, Wise TN: Psychological factors affecting medical condition: a new proposal for DSM-V. Psychosomatics. 2007, 48: 103-111. 10.1176/appi.psy.48.2.103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Barsky AJ, Goodson JD, Lane RS, Cleary PD: The amplification of somatic symptoms. Psychosom Med. 1988, 50: 510-519.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Barsky AJ: Amplification, somatization, and the somatoform disorders. Psychosomatics. 1992, 33: 28-34.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Nakao M, Barsky AJ, Kumano H, Kuboki T: Relationship between somatosensory amplification and alexithymia in a Japanese psychosomatic clinic. Psychosomatics. 2002, 43: 55-60. 10.1176/appi.psy.43.1.55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Barsky AJ, Wyshak G, Klerman GL: The somatosensory amplification scale and its relationship to hypochondriasis. J Psychiatr Res. 1990, 24: 323-334. 10.1016/0022-3956(90)90004-A.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Nakao M, Kumano H, Kuboki T, Barsky AJ: Reliability and validity of the Japanese Version of Somatosensory Amplification Scale: clinical application to psychosomatic illness. Jpn J Psychosom Med. 2001, 41: 539-547. in Japanese

    Google Scholar 

  13. Gulec H, Sayar K: Reliability and validity of the Turkish form of the Somatosensory Amplification Scale. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2007, 61: 25-30. 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2007.01606.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Nakao M, Barsky AJ, Nishikitani M, Yano E, Murata K: Somatosensory amplification and its relationship to somatosensory, auditory, and visual evoked and event-related potentials (P300). Neurosci Lett. 2007, 415: 185-189. 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.01.021.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Nakao M, Tamiya N, Yano E: Gender and somatosensory amplification in relation to perceived work stress and social support in Japanese workers. Women Health. 2005, 42: 41-54. 10.1300/J013v42n01_03.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB, Linzer M, Hahn SR, deGruy FV, Brody D: Physical symptoms in primary care. Predictors of psychiatric disorders and functional impairment. Arch Fam Med. 1994, 3: 774-779. 10.1001/archfami.3.9.774.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Sifneos PE: The prevalence of alexithymia characteristics in psychosomatic patients. Psychother Psychosom. 1973, 22: 255-262.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Lesser IM: Current concepts in psychiatry: alexithymia. N Engl J Med. 1985, 312: 690-692.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Taylor GJ, Parker JD, Bagby RM, Acklin MW: Alexithymia and somatic complaints in psychiatric out-patients. J Psychosom Res. 1992, 36: 417-424. 10.1016/0022-3999(92)90002-J.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Wise TN, Mann LS: The relationship between somatosensory amplification, alexithymia, and neuroticism. J Psychosom Res. 1994, 38: 515-521. 10.1016/0022-3999(94)90048-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Taylor GJ: Recent developments in alexithymia theory and research. Can J Psychiatry. 2000, 45: 134-142.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Kosturek A, Gregory RJ, Sousou AJ, Trief P: Alexithymia and somatic amplification in chronic pain. Psychosomatics. 1998, 39: 399-404.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Jones MP, Schettler A, Olden K, Crowell MD: Alexithymia and somatosensory amplification in functional dyspepsia. Psychosomatics. 2004, 45: 508-516. 10.1176/appi.psy.45.6.508.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Duddu V, Isaac MK, Chaturvedi SK: Somatization, somatosensory amplification, attribution styles and illness behaviour: a review. Int Rev Psychiatry. 2006, 18: 25-33. 10.1080/09540260500466790.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Aronson KR, Barrett LF, Quigley KS: Feeling your body or feeling badly: evidence for the limited validity of the Somatosensory Amplification Scale as an index of somatic sensitivity. J Psychosom Res. 2001, 51: 387-394. 10.1016/S0022-3999(01)00216-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Fabbri S, Kapur N, Wells A, Creed F: Emotional, cognitive, and behavioral characteristics of medical outpatients: a preliminary analysis. Psychosomatics. 2001, 42: 74-77. 10.1176/appi.psy.42.1.74.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Sayar K, Barsky AJ, Gulec H: Does somatosensory amplification decrease with antidepressant treatment?. Psychosomatics. 2005, 46: 340-344. 10.1176/appi.psy.46.4.340.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Mailloux J, Brener J: Somatosensory amplification and its relationship to heartbeat detection ability. Psychosom Med. 2002, 64: 353-357.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Aronson KR, Barrett LF, Quigley K: Emotional reactivity and the overreport of somatic symptoms: somatic sensitivity or negative reporting style?. J Psychosom Res. 2006, 60: 521-530. 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2005.09.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Murata K, Araki S, Okajima F, Nakao M, Suwa K, Matsunaga C: Effects of occupational use of vibrating tools in the autonomic, central and peripheral nervous system. Int Arch Occup Environ Health. 1997, 70: 94-100. 10.1007/s004200050192.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Murata K, Araki S, Yokoyama K, Okumura T, Ishimatsu S, Takasu N, White RF: Asymptomatic sequelae to acute sarin poisoning in the central and autonomic nervous system 6 months after the Tokyo subway attack. J Neurol. 1997, 244: 601-606. 10.1007/s004150050153.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Ferguson E, Swairbrick R, Clare S, Robinson E, Bignell CJ, Anderson C: Hypochondriacal concerns, somatosensory amplification, and primary and secondary cognitive appraisals. Br J Med Psychol. 2000, 73: 355-369. 10.1348/000711200160561.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Barsky AJ, Ahern DK: Cognitive behavior therapy for hypochondriasis: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2004, 291: 1464-1470. 10.1001/jama.291.12.1464.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Speckens AE, Spinhoven P, Sloekers PP, Bolk JH, van Hemert AM: A validation study of the Whitely Index, the Illness Attitude Scales, and the Somatosensory Amplification Scale in general medical and general practice patients. J Psychosom Res. 1996, 40: 95-104. 10.1016/0022-3999(95)00561-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Speckens AE, Van Hemert AM, Spinhoven P, Bolk JH: The diagnostic and prognostic significance of the Whitely Index, the Illness Attitude Scales and the Somatosensory Amplification Scale. Psychol Med. 1996, 26: 1085-1090.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Avia MD: The development of illness beliefs. J Psychosom Res. 1999, 47: 199-204. 10.1016/S0022-3999(99)00037-9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Suzuki M, Gyoba J, Kano M: Analyzing the aesthetic impressions of alexithymic Japanese students. Psychol Rep. 2004, 94: 669-682. 10.2466/PR0.94.2.669-682.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Brown RJ, Poliakoff E, Kirkman MA: Somatoform dissociation and somatosensory amplification are differentially associated with attention to the tactile modality following exposure to body-related stimuli. J Psychosom Res. 2007, 62: 159-165. 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2006.08.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Kano M, Hamaguchi T, Itoh M, Yanai K, Fukudo S: Correlation between alexithymia and hypersensitivity to visceral stimulation in human. Pain. 2007, Epub 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Wise TN, Mann LS: The attribution of somatic symptoms in psychiatric outpatients. Compr Psychiatry. 1995, 36: 407-410. 10.1016/S0010-440X(95)90247-3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Spinhoven P, van der Does AJ: Somatization and somatosensory amplification in psychiatric outpatients: an explorative study. Compr Psychiatry. 1997, 38: 93-97. 10.1016/S0010-440X(97)90087-0.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Porcelli P: Psychological abnormalities in patients with irritable bowel syndrome. Indian J Gastroenterol. 2004, 23: 63-69.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Jones MP, Wessinger S, Crowell MD: Coping strategies and interpersonal support in patients with irritable bowel syndrome and inflammatory bowel disease. Clin Gastroenterol Hepatol. 2006, 4: 474-481. 10.1016/j.cgh.2005.12.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Raphael KG, Marbach JJ, Gallagher RM: Somatosensory amplification and affective inhibition are elevated in myofascial face pain. Pain Med. 2000, 1: 247-253. 10.1046/j.1526-4637.2000.00034.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Gregory RJ, Manring J, Berry SL: Pain location and psychological characteristics of patients with chronic pain. Psychosomatics. 2000, 41: 216-220. 10.1176/appi.psy.41.3.216.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Gregory RJ, Manring J, Wade MJ: Personality traits related to chronic pain location. Ann Clin Psychiatry. 2005, 17: 59-64. 10.1080/10401230590932317.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Barsky AJ, Wyshak G: Hypochondriasis and somatosensory amplification. Br J Psychiatry. 1990, 157: 404-409.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Barsky AJ, Wyshak G, Klerman GL: Transient hypochondriasis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1990, 47: 746-752.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Haenen MA, Schmidi AJ, Schoenmakers M, van den Hout MA: Tactual sensitivity in hypochondriasis. Psychother Psychosom. 1997, 66: 128-132.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Duddu V, Chaturvedi SK, Isaac MK: Amplification and attribution styles in somatoform and depressive disorders: a study from Bangalore, India. Psychopathology. 2003, 36: 98-103. 10.1159/000070365.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Witthoft M, Gerlach AL, Bailer J: Selective attention, memory bias, and symptom perception in idiopathic environmental intolerance and somatoform disorders. J Abnorm Psychol. 2006, 115: 397-407. 10.1037/0021-843X.115.3.397.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Bailer J, Witthoft M, Bayerl C, Rist F: Syndrome stability and psychological predictors of symptom severity in idiopathic environmental intolerance and somatoform disorders. Psychol Med. 2007, 37: 271-281. 10.1017/S0033291706009354.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Stewart SH, Watt MC: Illness Attitudes Scale dimensions and their associations with anxiety-related constructs in a nonclinical sample. Behav Res Ther. 2000, 38: 83-99. 10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00207-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Marcus DK, Gurley JR, Marchi MM, Bauer C: Cognitive and perceptual variables in hypochondriasis and health anxiety: a systematic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2007, 27: 127-139. 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.09.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Sayar K, Kirmayer LJ, Taillefer SS: Predictors of somatic symptoms in depressive disorder. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 2003, 25: 108-114. 10.1016/S0163-8343(02)00277-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Nakao M, Kashiwagi M, Yano E: Alexithymia and grief reactions in bereaved Japanese women. Death Stud. 2005, 29: 423-433. 10.1080/07481180590932535.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Torosian T, Lumley MA, Pickard SD, Ketterer MW: Silent versus symptomatic myocardial ischemia: the role of psychological and medical factors. Health Psychol. 1997, 16: 123-130. 10.1037/0278-6133.16.2.123.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Delle Chiaie R, Baciarello G, Villani M, Iannucci G, Regine F, Didonna A, Talamonti F, Pancheri P: Cardiovascular reactivity of mitral valve prolapse patients during experimental stress exposure: evidence for a functional nature of cardiovascular symptoms. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1996, 93: 434-441. 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1996.tb10674.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Spinhoven P, Verschuur M: Predictors of fatigue in rescue workers and residents in the aftermath of an aviation disaster: a longitudinal study. Psychosom Med. 2006, 68: 605-612. 10.1097/01.psy.0000222367.88642.de.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Muramatsu K, Miyaoka H, Muramatsu Y, Fuse K, Yoshimine F, Kamijima K, Gejyo F, Sakurai K: The amplification of somatic symptoms in upper respiratory tract infections. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 2002, 24: 172-175. 10.1016/S0163-8343(02)00177-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Wyshak G, Barsky AJ, Klerman GL: Comparison of psychiatric screening tests in a general medical setting using ROC analysis. Med Care. 1991, 29: 775-785. 10.1097/00005650-199108000-00010.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Stewart DE, Reicher AE, Gerulath AH, Boydell KM: Vulvodynia and psychological distress. Obstet Gynecol. 1994, 84: 587-590.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Von Korff M, Simon G: The relationship between pain and depression. Br J Psychiatry Suppl. 1996, 30: 101-108.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Ferguson E: Hypochondriacal concerns and the five factor model of personality. J Pers. 2000, 68: 705-724. 10.1111/1467-6494.00113.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Haenen MA, de Jong PJ, Schmidt AJ, Stevens S, Visser L: Hypochondriacs' estimation of negative outcomes: domain-specificity and responsiveness to reassuring and alarming information. Behav Res Ther. 2000, 38: 819-833. 10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00128-X.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Gregory RJ: Characteristics of patients assigned multiple nonthreatening medical diagnoses. Prim Care Companion J Clin Psychiatry. 2001, 3: 164-167.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Perme B, Ranjith G, Mohan R, Chandrasekaran R: Dhat (semen loss) syndrome: a functional somatic syndrome of the Indian subcontinent?. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 2005, 27: 215-217. 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2005.01.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Jones MP, Roth LM, Crowell MD: Symptom reporting by functional dyspeptics during the water load test. Am J Gastroenterol. 2005, 100: 1334-1339. 10.1111/j.1572-0241.2005.40802.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Barsky AJ, Borus JF: Functional somatic syndromes. Ann Intern Med. 1999, 130: 910-921.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Barsky AJ: Clinical practice. The patient with hypochondriasis. N Engl J Med. 2001, 345: 1395-1399. 10.1056/NEJMcp002896.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Nakao M, Myers P, Fricchione G, Zuttermeister PC, Barsky AJ, Benson H: Somatization and symptom reduction through a behavioral medicine intervention in a mind/body medicine clinic. Behav Med. 2001, 26: 169-176.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mutsuhiro Nakao.

Additional information

Competing interests

The author(s) declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

MN wrote the first draft of the paper, and AJB revised it critically for important intellectual content. Both authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Nakao, M., Barsky, A.J. Clinical application of somatosensory amplification in psychosomatic medicine. BioPsychoSocial Med 1, 17 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1186/1751-0759-1-17

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1751-0759-1-17

Keywords